AMPA Receptor

From bradwiki
Revision as of 16:28, 24 April 2015 by Bradley Monk (talk | contribs)
Jump to navigation Jump to search


Surface & Synaptic Expression of GluR Subtypes

Hayashi Malinow • 2000 • Science • PDF




Articles

Quick Facts

  • Most AMPARs in adult hippocampus and cortex appear to consist of the GluA1 plus GluA2, or GluA2 plus GluA3 subunits. Since GluA3 is expressed at relatively low levels, over 70% of GluA2 is associated with GluA1
  • All AMPAR subunits consist of an extracellular N-terminal and a intracellular C-terminal
  • GluA2 has a positively charged arginine (R) in the M2 channel-forming segment
  • GluA1 and the other non-GluA2 receptors have a glutamine (Q) in the M2 channel-forming segment
  • Edited GluA2 R-subunits remain largely unassembled and are retained at the endoplasmic reticulum, whereas unedited GluA1 Q-subunits readily tetramerize and traffic to synapses.
  • GluA2 with positively charged arginine (R) in the channel-forming segment blocks calcium entry and results in AMPARs with low conductance and a linear current–voltage relationship
  • GluA1 with unedited glutamine (Q) subunits are Ca2+ permeable, have high conductance, and are susceptible to voltage-dependent block by endogenous polyamines
  • That Q/R editing is essential for correct brain function and is demonstrated by the finding that mice engineered to synthesize only unedited GluA2 subunits die early and develop seizures, the same goes for ADAR2 knockout (KO) mice who also have seizures


LTP Dynamics

AMPARs
  • GluA2 blocks calcium
  • GluA1 permits calcium
  • GluA2 trumps GluA1 (GluA2 is dominant phenotype)
Glutamate causes
  • increased AMPAR synaptic diffusion rates
  • rapid AMPAR endocytosis
Calcium causes
  • decreased AMPAR diffusion rates
  • increased AMPAR synaptic expression
  • blocking calcium increases AMPAR diffusion
  • Since GluA2 blocks calcium, it's presence in a synapse may destabilize the local potentiation status.
AMPARs vs. NMDAR diffusion
  • extrasynaptic: AMPAR 4x faster than NMDARs
    • (10.0 vs 2.3 nm/s)
  • synaptic: similar speeds
    • (28.0 vs 21.0 nm/s)
  • Note that diffusion was faster outside the synapse than inside the synapse (that doesn't make sense)
Neural Stimulation with KCl causes
  • extrasynaptic AMPARs to diffuse 5x faster than baseline
  • synaptic AMPAR mobility didn't change
PKC activity (stimulated by TPA) causes
  • extrasynaptic: AMPAR and NMDAR have significantly increased mobility
  • synaptic: AMPAR and NMDAR have significantly increased mobility




Qual Qual Journals Quantum Dots AMPA Receptor SAP


Bassani · Folci · Zapata · Passafaro 2013

AMPAR trafficking in synapse maturation and plasticity

Abstract

Glutamate ionotropic AMPA receptors (AMPAR) mediate most fast excitatory synaptic transmission in the central nervous system. The content and composition of AMPARs in postsynaptic membranes (which determine synaptic strength) are dependent on the regulated trafficking of AMPAR subunits in and out of the membranes. AMPAR trafficking is a key mechanism that drives nascent synapse development, and is the main determinant of both Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity in mature synapses. Hebbian plasticity seems to be the biological substrate of at least some forms of learning and memory; while homeostatic plasticity (also known as synaptic scaling) keeps neuronal circuits stable by maintaining changes within a physiological range

Introduction

Glutamatergic synapses mediate excitatory transmission in the central nervous system. AMPARs and NMDARs are the two principal types of ionotropic glutamate receptors in glutamatergic synapses, and changes in the trafficking, subunit composition, and signaling of these receptors are fundamental processes underlying synapse strength. AMPARs are tetrameric (four subunit) cation channels that mediate fast excitatory synaptic transmission. NMDARs are also tetrameric cation channels; their activation requires glutamate (ligand-gating) and also membrane depolarization (voltage dependence), which removes the Mg2+ normally blocking the channel. Activated NMDARs allow Ca2+ to enter the neuron; the magnitude of the Ca2+ signal in the postsynaptic neuron largely determines whether long-term potentiation (LTP) or long-term depression (LTD) of AMPAR currents occurs.


In mature synapses, AMPAR trafficking is a major determinant of both Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity. Hebbian plasticity is a mechanism by which long-lasting modifications in synaptic strength occur [4 ]. Synaptic modifications resulting from Hebbian plasticity give rise to LTP and LTD and are largely determined by the delivery and removal of postsynaptic AMPARs. There is good evidence that LTP and LTD are substrates for at least some aspects of learning, memory, and cognition [5 ]; and some diseases characterized by defective learning and cognitive function are directly related to alterations in AMPAR trafficking [6 ]. Homeostatic synaptic plasticity on the other hand is a mechanism ensuring that changes in synaptic activity occur within a limited range, thereby preserving the stability of neuronal circuits and functional integrity of the brain [7 ].

AMPAR structure and subunit composition

AMPARs are homo or hetero tetramers assembled from GluA1-A4 subunits, encoded by genes GRIA1-4 [8 ]. The four subunits that make up each receptor combine in various stoichiometries to form receptor subtypes with distinct channel properties [9 ]. Most AMPARs in adult hippocampus and cortex appear to consist of the GluA1 plus GluA2, or GluA2 plus GluA3 subunits [10 ]. Since GluA3 is expressed at relatively low levels, over 70 % of GluA2 is associated with GluA1 [11 ]. All AMPAR subunits consist of an extracellular amino terminal domain (ATD or NTD), a ligand-binding domain (LBD) (S1 and S2), three membrane-spanning domains (M1, M2 and M3), one cytoplasmic re-entrant loop (P), and a carboxy-terminal intracellular region (Fig. 1) [9, 12, 13]. The extracellular and transmembrane regions of all GluAs are highly homologous and the four GluAs differ from each other mainly in terms of their intracellular cytoplasmic tails. The function of the NTD in AMPARs is to control the initial dimerization of subunits and to prevent heteromerization between AMPAR and kainate receptor subunits [14]. Subsequent tetramerization of dimers is mediated by associations at the ligand binding and membrane domains (M1–M3 and P) and also depends on Q/R editing of GluA2 (see below) [15]. GluA1, GluA4, and GluA2L (alternative splice form of GluA2) have long cytoplasmic tails; GluA2, GluA3, and GluA4S (alternative splice form of GluA4) have short C-terminal tails [16, 17, 18]. Alternative splicing also generates the flop (short) and flip (long) variants encoded by exons 14 and 15, respectively, that differ by a 38-amino-acid insertion into a region that forms part of the extracellular LBD and is localized before the M3 domain [19, 20]. The flip variants of all subunits are prominently expressed before birth and their expression, as determined by in situ hybridization, remains largely unchanged during postnatal development and in the adult; whereas the expression of flop variants increases throughout development, and reaches adult levels by postnatal day 14 in the rat [21]. Flop versions are less responsive to AMPAR potentiators [21, 22] and generally de-sensitize more rapidly in response to glutamate than receptors containing flip variant [19].


Almost all (99 %) GluA2s in adult brain have a positively charged arginine (R) in the M2 channel-forming segment at position 607 (Q/R site), while the other AMPAR subunits have glutamine (Q) at this position [15 ]. This is not due to a primary coding difference, but to site-selective deamination of adenosine to inosine on the pre-mRNA, which changes the glutamine codon to an arginine codon [23 ]. Deamination is performed by ADAR2, a double-stranded-RNA-specific adenosine deaminase and principal RNA-editing enzyme in mammals; ADAR2 can regulate its own expression and activity by editing its own pre-mRNA [24 ]. Unedited (Q) GluA2 exists during embryogenesis where it seems to have the important role of directing human neural progenitor cell differentiation to neurons [25 ]. Edited R-containing subunits remain largely unassembled and are retained at the endoplasmic reticulum, whereas unedited Q subunits readily tetramerize and traffic to synapses. Furthermore, the presence of a positively charged amino acid (R) in the channel-forming segment effectively blocks calcium entry and results in AMPARs with relatively low conductance and a linear current–voltage relationship [26 ]. By contrast, AMPARs containing unedited subunits are Ca2+ permeable, have higher conductance, and are susceptible to voltage-dependent block by endogenous intracellular polyamines [26 ]. Thus, this single amino acid residue not only affects channel composition but also controls ion conduction and channel rectification as well as subunit retention at the endoplasmic reticulum [15 ]. That Q/R editing is essential for correct brain function and is demonstrated by the finding that mice engineered to synthesize only unedited GluA2 subunits [27 , 28 , 29 ] die early and develop seizures, as do ADAR2 knockout (KO) mice [23 ].

AMPAR Interacting Proteins

AMPAR trafficking and synaptic targeting relies on interactions with several types of proteins, comprising those that interact with the extracellular N-terminal domain; those that interact with the intracellular C-terminal domain (including proteins with PDZ domains), and the so-called AMPAR auxiliary proteins.

Proteins interacting with the extracellular domain

The first two proteins demonstrated to interact with all four AMPA receptor subunits were neuronal pentraxin 1 (NP1) and neuronal immediate early gene neuronal activity-regulated pentraxin (NARP) [30 ]. NP1 induces clustering of GluA4 homomeric receptors—the main AMPARs expressed during synaptogenesis [30 , 31 ] whereas NARP induces clustering of GluA1-, GluA2-, and GluA3-containing AMPARs in both neurons and heterologous cells. Experimental overexpression of the gene encoding NARP in neurons increases the number of synaptic AMPAR clusters [32 ], suggesting that NARP may be important for stabilizing synaptic AMPAR clusters at excitatory synapses. The extracellular domain of GluA2 is also the specific site for interaction with N-cadherin—an interaction important for the formation, growth, and maintenance of dendritic spines. The first evidence of this was the finding that GluA2 overexpression in mature cultured hippocampal neurons increased spine length, spine head width, and spine density, and that this activity required NTD [33 ]. GluA2 may therefore stimulate synaptic development and dendritic spine formation via this novel structural interaction at the synaptic junction [34 ] (Fig. 2 ).

Proteins interacting with the intracellular domain
  • PDZ domain proteins interacting with the C-terminal domain

The C-terminus of GluA1 binds with a PDZ domain of SAP97—a member of the membrane-associated guanylate kinase (MAGUK) family [35 ]. SAP97 interacts with the protein kinase A (PKA) anchoring molecule AKAP79 [36 ], which may serve to enhance the GluA1 phosphorylation required for LTP [37 ]. Recently, it has been shown that neuronal activity induces AKAP79/150 palmitoylation, which is required for AKAP79/150 recruitment at spines and for spine enlargement [38 , 39 ]. In AKAP79/150-KO mice, PKA does not reach the postsynaptic membrane and the bidirectional modulation of postsynaptic AMPARs is altered, with concomitant alterations in synaptic transmission and memory [40 ]. However, SAP97 has also been shown to act early in the secretory pathway to facilitate AMPAR maturation [41 ]. Furthermore, while some studies have found that SAP97 overexpression in cultured neurons increased the number of synaptic AMPARs and NMDARs [42 , 43 ], other studies report that SAP97 has no significant effect on excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) mediated by AMPAR or NMDAR [44 , 45 ]. The situation is further complicated by the recent finding that SAP97 conditional KO mice have normal LTP [46 ]. Clearly, further research is required to elucidate the roles of SAP97 in AMPAR trafficking. GluA2 and GluA3 share a C-terminal sequence (-SVKI) that interacts with glutamate receptor-interacting proteins (GRIP1/2) containing seven PDZ domains [47 ]; with AMPAR binding protein (ABP) [48 , 49 ], which has six PDZ domains; and with PICK1, which contains a single PDZ domain. ABP seems to be a splice variant of GRIP2 (also called GRIPrelated protein), which lacks the GRIP2 N-terminus and PDZ7 [48 ]. ABP and GRIP are found at the PSD and also in intracellular punctate structures resembling endosomes. Several studies have implicated GRIP1/2 in the control of AMPAR trafficking, as well as synaptic plasticity and social behavior. For example, genetic ablation of GRIP1/2 abolishes cerebellar LTD [50 ] and affected mice show increased sociability and impaired prepulse inhibition [51 ]. However, it is unclear how GRIP1/2 achieves this control. Some studies suggest that AMPAR delivery to dendrites and synapses requires GRIP1 interaction with kinesin heavy chain [52 ] and liprin-α , which in turn binds microtubule-based motor KIF1A [53 , 54 ] and also the LAR family of tyrosine phosphatase receptors [55 ]. Liprin-α has been shown to be important for both postsynaptic and presynaptic maturation [53 ]. Other studies indicate that GRIP1 retains AMPARs in the intracellular compartment [56 ]. Others again indicate that GRIP1 regulates the endosomal recycling of AMPARs in that GRIP1 binding to neuronenriched endosomal protein 21 kDa (NEEP21) promotes the recycling of internalized AMPARs back to the plasma membrane [57 , 58 ]. It has also been reported that GRIP1 binds KIF5, another microtubule-based motor protein important for vesicular transport along axons and dendrites: the KIF5–GRIP1 complex interacts with GluA2, and appears to be involved in the transport of GluA2 to dendrites [52 ]. The interaction of GluA2 with PICK1 is important for GluA2 endocytosis [59 ]. Furthermore, the interactions of GRIP/ABP and PICK1 with GluA2 depend on the phosphorylation status of serine 880 (S880) and tyrosine 876 (Y876) on the C-terminal of the GluA2 subunit: In particular, phosphorylation of S880 prevents the interaction of GRIP/ABP with GluA2, but not of PICK1 with GluA2 [60 , 61 ]. On the other hand, ABP binding can itself prevent S880 phosphorylation [62 ]. Phosphorylation of Y876 by Src tyrosine kinase also regulates the GRIP/ABP interaction with GluA2, but not the PICK1 interaction [63 ]. GRIP, ABP/GRIP2, and PICK also play critical roles in LTD and are involved in regulating the recycling of internalized following NMDAR activation [64 ], in association with the memory performance-associated protein KIBRA, which also binds PICK1 and AMPAR [65 ]. Recently, PICK1 has been shown to interact with tetraspannin- 7 (Tspan7), a protein involved in X-linked intellectual disability, and this interaction has been shown to be important for the localization of AMPARs at the postsynaptic membrane, and for synapse maturation [66 ].

  • Interactions of with 4.1N, NSF, and AP2 with the C-terminal domain

The GluA1 subunit forms a complex with 4.1N—a neuronal specific form of 4.1R, the red blood cell actin cytoskeletonassociated protein—as a result of which AMPARs appear to be stabilized at the cell surface [67 ]. NSF is an ATPase known to play an important role in membrane fusion [68 , 69 , 70 ]. It has also been found that NSF interacts with the C-terminus of GluA2 and that this interaction is required for the insertion of GluA2-containing AMPARs into the membrane, and their stabilization there [68 , 71 , 72 , 73 , 74 , 75 ]. Disruption of the NSF–GluA2 interaction by specific peptides causes a rundown of EPSCs [68 , 70 , 71 , 74 ], while a mutated GluA2 that does not interact with NSF does not arrive at synapses in hippocampal slice cultures [76 ]. It has been shown that inhibition of NSF activity prevents LTP, while the amount of NSF in the PSD appears to be regulated dynamically [77 ]. AP2 is a clathrin adaptor complex involved in endocytosis. It interacts with GluA2 at a binding site in the C-terminal region overlapping (but not identical to) the NSF binding site [71 , 78 ] and this interaction seems to be important for clathrin-mediated endocytosis during NMDA receptor-mediated LTD [71 ]. One study that used a peptide to block the AP2–GluA2 interaction found increased AMPAR-mediated transmission [78 ]; however, an earlier study that used another peptide to block this interaction had no effect on basal transmission but selectively prevented LTD [71 ]. Further study is required to clarify the role of AP2 in AMPAR trafficking, although it does seem clear that defective AP2–GluA2 interaction interferes with LTD.

  • AMPA auxiliary proteins

The AMPA auxiliary proteins are another group of molecules important for AMPAR targeting to the postsynaptic membrane. They include the transmembrane AMPAR regulatory proteins (TARPs), cornichon-like proteins, neuropilin, and tolloid-like proteins [79 , 80 , 81 , 82 , 83 , 84 ]. TARPs regulate AMPAR trafficking and channel kinetics and are classified into six isoforms—type 1a (γ -2/stargazin and γ -3), type 1b (γ -4 and γ -8), and type 2 (γ -5 and γ -7)— according to how they control AMPAR trafficking and channel properties [85 , 86 ]. They stabilize AMPARs at the postsynaptic membrane by direct interaction with scaffolding protein PSD95 and other MAGUKs. TARPs also slow deactivation and desensitization, thereby increasing AMPAR conductance, and influencing AMPAR affinity for pharmacological agents [85 , 87 , 88 , 89 , 90 ]. Synaptic TARP phosphorylation is activity regulated and in turn phosphorylation influences stargazin binding to PSD-95 [91 , 92 , 93 ]. CaMKIIdependent phosphorylation of stargazin retains AMPARs at postsynaptic sites by reducing AMPAR diffusion [94 ]. Interaction of AMPARs with the cornichon-like proteins CNIH2 and CNIH3 was recently reported by Schwenk et al. [95 ], who showed that this interaction is involved in the regulation of AMPAR expression in the postsynaptic membrane, and also influences AMPAR channel properties. CNIH2 and CNIH3 bind AMPARs which in turn complex with TARPγ 4 in the hippocampus [96 ]. In this complex, the cornichon-like proteins control AMPAR trafficking to the postsynaptic membrane, whereas TARPγ 4 stabilizes the CNIH2/3–AMPAR interaction in the membrane [97 ].

AMPAR trafficking at nascent versus mature synapses

As noted above, the subunit composition, channel properties, and membrane protein interactions of glutamate receptors change during the transition from the nascent to the mature synapse [98 , 99 ]. Some of these changes are summarized in Fig. 2 . It was unclear for some time whether newborn synapses express AMPARs, and whether AMPAR recruitment occurs at the same time as NMDAR recruitment, or comes later. However, evidence now indicates that AMPARs are present at the very beginning of synapse formation. In particular, it has been shown that surface AMPARs are expressed by neuronal progenitors and are also functional [100 , 101 , 102 , 103 ]. It has also been shown in rodents that in the first postnatal week, spontaneous activity induces the delivery of GluA4-containing AMPARs to the postsynaptic membranes of hippocampal neurons, and that these AMPARs mediate fast excitatory transmission there [104 ]. In mature synapses, GluA4-containing AMPARs are substituted by those containing GluA2, enabling long-term maintenance of synaptic strength [104 ]. These recent findings are consistent with the model proposed by Groc et al. [105 ], which posits that AMPARs are present in the nascent synapse both in a labile state, which is highly sensitive to synaptic activity, and also in a silent state, insensitive to such activity. Not only does AMPAR subunit composition vary during synapse maturation but the mechanism by which AMPARs are recruited to the synapse also changes. Thus, AMPARs are delivered to mature postsynaptic membranes by a mechanism requiring NMDAR activation; while at nascent synapses, AMPARs are recruited independently of NMDAR signaling [105 ]. Furthermore, in mature synapses, NMDAR activation inhibits AMPAR signaling by regulating both subunit composition and membrane stabilization (reviewed in [3 , 106 ]). NMDAR composition also changes during development. NMDARs are formed by the association of GluN1 and GluN2 subunits (GluN2A–D), and in nascent synapses, GluN1 and GluN2B are mainly present. During maturation, there is a switch from GluN2B to GluN2A subunits [107 ], which is fundamental for determining mature NMDAR kinetics and signaling pathways. GluN2B-containing NMDARs down-regulate AMPARs at synapses via negative regulation of TARP expression, as shown by studies in which GluN2B was genetically ablated [3 ]. In nascent synapses, NMDAR activation also inhibits the transcription and translation of AMPAR GluA1 and GluA2 subunits and promotes the degradation of the AMPAR-binding partners GRIP1 and PSD95, thereby preventing AMPAR stabilization in the synapse (reviewed in [106 ]). This mechanism probably accounts for the low AMPAR/NMDAR ratio in nascent synapses, although this ratio increases progressively during synapse maturation. In fact, a key aspect of synapse maturation is the change in the relative contribution of AMPARs and NMDARs to synaptic currents [108 ]. Other proteins that are involved in the transition from immature to mature synapses include postsynaptic scaffolding proteins, particularly MAGUKs, SAP102, and PSD95 [109 , 110 , 111 ]. Whereas SAP102 regulates AMPA and NMDA receptor trafficking during synaptogenesis, PSD95 plays a central role in their trafficking at mature synapses [43 , 44 , 45 , 112 ]. During synapse maturation, PSD95 regulates the developmental switch between GluN2B and GluN2A NMDAR subunits and promotes the increase in AMPAR transmission [113 ].

AMPAR trafficking in Hebbian plasticity

As noted, Hebbian plasticity is a mechanism giving rise to long-lasting modifications in synaptic strength, including LTP and LTD [4 ]. The mechanism may be considered to start by presynaptic depolarization resulting in sufficient glutamate to activate the NMDA receptor, which, as a result, becomes permeable to Ca2+ so that the intracellular concentration of Ca2+ increases. A strong but brief increase in postsynaptic Ca2+ promotes LTP, whereas sustained lowlevel Ca2+ elevation induces LTD [114 , 115 , 116 , 117 , 118 ]. Intracellular Ca2+ exerts it effects via several signaling pathways including those involving protein kinases, for example Ca2+ -calmodulin-dependent protein kinases I and II (CaMKI and CaMKII), cAMP-dependent PKA, and protein kinase C (PKC); and those involving protein phosphatases such as protein phosphatases 1 (PP1), 2A (PP2A), and 2B (PP2B). It is these signaling pathways that effect the changes contributing to LTP and LTD. Thus, they regulate AMPAR trafficking to and from the postsynaptic membrane [114 ], they influence AMPAR subunit composition at the membrane over the longer term (reviewed in [119 ]); and they effect short-term post-translational modifications (phosphorylation and dephosphorylation) of pre-existing AMPARs to change channel permeability [120 ] and modulate interactions between AMPAR subunits and other proteins (to in turn affect AMPAR trafficking and stabilization) [17 ]. Figure 3 summarizes the principal AMPAR trafficking events, to and from the postsynaptic membrane, that occur during LTP and LTD.

LTP is characterized by long-lasting potentiation of AMPAR-mediated EPSCs [121 ]. Current increase is mediated by post-translational modifications to AMPARs in the early phase of LTP, and by the production of new AMPARs in the late phase [121 , 122 , 123 ]. In the initial phase of LTP, GluA1-containing AMPARs are recruited to the synapse [76 , 124 , 125 ] (Fig. 3 ). According to the widely accepted model, GluA1–GluA2 receptors are excluded from synapses unless an LTP stimulus is provided, whereas GluA2–GluA3 receptors traffic to the synapse constitutively. This difference in trafficking behavior is thought to be mediated by the C-tails of individual subunit proteins [126 ]. Evidence in favor of this model is that LTP is impaired in GluA1-KO mice [127 ] and that LTP is normal in GluA2–GluA3 double KO mice [126 ]. Furthermore, impairment depends on age, so that GluA1- KO mice show LTP when young, but by postnatal day 42, LTP has declined to very low levels [128 ]. The occurrence of LTP in young GluA1-KO animals indicates that a GluA1- independent form of LTP can occur when GluA1-containing AMPARs are not available [128 ]. This is further supported by the recent finding that LTP can occur “normally” in neurons in which the genes for GluA1, GluA2, and GluA3 have been deleted [129 ]. Conflicting results have been obtained regarding the role of GluA1 C-terminal tail interactions with PDZdomain proteins in LTP. When the GluA1 C-terminus fragment was overexpressed [76 ] or the PDZ-binding motif of GluA1 mutated [130 ], GluA1 delivery to the synapse was impaired and LTP prevented in CA1 pyramidal neurons in slice culture [76 , 130 ]. However, an in vivo study in knockin mice with truncated GluA1 found that LTP was induced in the absence of the GluA1 PDZ ligand motif [131 ]. These discrepant findings would appear to be model-dependent. The role of the GluA1 subunit in LTP was further investigated by Selcher et al. [132 ] using a mouse strain lacking this subunit. Their data indicated that GluA1 was specifically recruited in LTP at both active (having both AMPARand NMDAR-mediated synaptic transmission) and silent synapses (lacking AMPAR-mediated transmission) [132 ]. By contrast, GluA1 loss did not impair LTD [132 ]. The situation is further complicated by the recent finding of Granger et al. [129 ] that the cytoplasmic tail of GluA1 is not required for AMPAR trafficking or, therefore LTP. In their experimental model—which deleted genes for GluA1, GluA2, and GluA3 and re-inserted them one at a time, often with C-tail mutations—they found that AMPARs made of any single subunit could efficiently mediate LTP, and that even kainate could do so. The only prerequisite for LTP in their system seemed to be the presence of a large reserve pool of glutamate receptors that could move to synapses. Of course their system may be too artificial: KO of the two or three principal AMPAR subunits may fundamentally alter the expression of the proteins that normally orchestrate AMPAR trafficking. Nevertheless, Granger and colleagues’ data do show that synapses can display remarkable adaptability, and should stimulate further studies to elucidate how AMPAR-mediated LTP is controlled. During the initiation of LTP, NMDARs are activated to enable the calcium influx [133 ] that is critical for the activation of kinases—notably CaMKII, PKC and PKA—that catalyze the phosphorylation of GluA1 [114 , 134 ], 135 , 136 , 137 ]. In turn, the phosphorylation state of the GluA1 subunit influences AMPAR insertion into the postsynaptic membrane. Four serine residues (Ser831, Ser845, Ser818, and Ser816) and one threonine residue (Thr840) on the GluA1 subunit can be phosphorylated [135 , 136 , 138 , 139 ]. Initially, only Ser831 (phosphorylated by CaMKII and PKC) and Ser845 (phosphorylated by PKA) were thought to be involved in LTP since phosphorylation at these sites is associated with AMPAR insertion at the postsynaptic membrane [140 , 141 ]. However, recent work has shown that Ser818 and Ser816 are also phosphorylated by PKC and that this phosphorylation also contributes to AMPAR insertion at the postsynaptic membrane during LTP [138 ]. Phosphorylation of these residues enhances the binding of GluA1 with protein 4.1N; by contrast the palmitoylation on cys-811 negatively regulates this interaction [142 ]. It was known that GluA1-4.1N binding regulates GluA1 surface expression [67 ], however, Lin et al. [142 ] reported that 4.1N is required for activity-dependent GluA1 insertion to extrasynaptic surface pools [142 ]. Extrasynaptic pools of AMPARs probably serve as a source of AMPARs for delivery to synapses during LTP, and replenishing these extrasynaptic AMPAR pools seems important for LTP maintenance. In fact, acute knockdown of 4.1N impairs LTP maintenance without affecting the initial phase of LTP [142 ]. Conversely, phosphorylation of Thr840 is associated with AMPAR removal and LTD [135 , 143 ]. Recently, another protein kinase, the atypical PKC isoform M zeta (PKMξ ), has been shown to be involved in LTP [144 ]. PKMξ increases AMPARs levels at the postsynaptic membrane via up-regulation of AMPAR trafficking that is dependent on the presence of GluA2 and also the N -ethylmaleimide- sensitive factor (NFS) [145 ]. Inhibition of PKMξ prevents the maintenance of LTP but has no effect on its induction [144 , 146 ]. Findings on the role of calcium permeable (CP-) AMPARs (lacking GluA2) in LTP are conflicting [147 , 148 , 149 , 150 , 151 ]. Plant et al. [147 ] reported that LTP in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons causes rapid and transient incorporation of GluA2-lacking receptors, which are subsequently replaced by GluA2-containing AMPARs. In agreement with these findings, Guire et al. [151 ] reported that the synaptic incorporation of CP-AMPARs is regulated by CaM-Kinase I; furthermore, this recruitment contributes to the actin-dependent structural plasticity induced by LTP [148 ]. Fortin et al. [148 ] also found that spine enlargement induced by GluA1 overexpression is associated with synaptic recruitment of CP AMPARs and increased mEPSCs; and also found that these events are blocked by IEM-1460—an agonist selective for CP-AMPARs—thus indicating that CP-AMPARs are involved in these events. However, Gray et al. [150 ] found that IEM-1460 blockage of GluA2-lacking AMPARs had no effect on LTP, indicating that LTP is unrelated to the insertion of GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors at the synapse. The data of Gray et al. [150 ] are also consistent with a study reporting that GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors are not inserted into synapses following the induction of LTP in hippocampal slices from young animals [149 ], suggesting the role of CP-AMPARs in LTP could be agedependent. Clearly the role of GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors in LTP still remains to be clarified. The mechanisms and routes by which AMPARs reach the synapse have not been completely elucidated. Correia et al. [152 ] investigated the short-range translocation of GluA1- containing AMPARs from the dendritic shaft to the spine. Their findings indicate that AMPAR-containing phosphorylated GluA1 is bound, via the adaptor protein Rab11, to the motor protein myosin Va, which seems to mediate AMPAR entry to the spine. Subsequent AMPAR delivery to the synapse occurs by two mechanisms: lateral diffusion within the spine membrane to the postsynaptic membrane [1 , 153 , 154 , 155 , 156 ] and exocytosis from recycling endosomes near [157 , 158 , 159 ] or at the synapse [160 ]. According to the three-step model of Choquet and colleagues, AMPARs of the intracellular pool are first inserted into the extra/perisynaptic surface, they then diffuse laterally to the postsynaptic membrane [1 , 153 , 154 , 155 , 156 ], and are retained there by interactions with scaffold proteins [161 ]. This model currently appears as the most plausible one accounting for AMPAR recruitment during LTP; however the order and importance of these steps, and in particular whether lateral movement of AMPARs precedes or follows membrane insertion, remain controversial. Thus, the presence of a pre-existing population of AMPARs at extrasynaptic membrane sites [1 , 162 ] constitutes indirect evidence for a “trapping first-exocytosis second” model in which recruitment of pre-existing extrasynaptic AMPARs to the postsynaptic membrane occurs first, and this followed by membrane insertion of newly recruited receptors to replenish those in extrasynaptic membrane pool [1 , 162 ]. Furthermore, AMPAR exocytosis occurs within minutes of NMDAR activation, a relatively slow time frame [153 , 159 ] in comparison to the instantaneous onset of LTP that occurs after such activation—supporting the idea that lateral movement of AMPARs is the initial step in LTP [163 ]. That exocytosis of recycled endosomes is important during the LTP is supported by the finding that the displacement of postsynaptic endocytic zones (Ezs) completely prevents LTP [164 ]. Direct evidence for activity-regulated AMPAR exocytosis has been obtained using high-resolution live cell imaging [157 ]. Exocytosis of AMPARs occurs in specific domains lateral to the PSD, which are enriched for the SNARE protein syntaxin-4 (Stx4). Stx4 appears to define a specific domain required for AMPAR exocytosis during synaptic potentiation, since disruption of Stx4 by a dominant-negative approach inhibited activity-induced AMPAR exocytosis and impaired LTP at hippocampal synapses [157 ]. To date, the precise location of activitydriven postsynaptic exocytosis remains controversial [141 , 153 , 154 , 157 , 158 , 159 , 160 ]. Complexins have recently emerged as important regulators of calcium-dependent AMPAR exocytosis in LTP [165 ]. Complexins are small synaptic proteins involved in neurotransmitter release [166 ] by promoting SNARE complex formation [167 ] and activation [168 , 169 ]. Mice lacking complexin-2 show impaired LTP [170 , 171 ]. Ahmad et al. [165 ] recently showed that these SNARE complex-binding proteins are required for postsynaptic exocytosis, which specifically occurs during LTP, although complexin1–2 was not shown to be required for basal exocytosis.

LTD is characterized by the loss of synaptic AMPARs triggered by activation of NMDAR or metabotropic glutamate receptors [114 ]. GluA2 appears to be a crucial mediator of AMPAR removal from the synapse in LTD [61 , 124 , 172 , 173 , 174 ]. Thus, loss of GluA2-containing AMPARs from the postsynaptic membrane has been observed under the confocal microscope in cultured neurons during LTD using GluA2 tagged with pHluorin, a pH-sensitive variant of GFP. Immediately after the addition of NMDA to initiate LTD, rapid endocytosis of extrasynaptic GluA2 was observed followed, after some delay, by removal of AMPARs from the postsynaptic membrane [175 ]. Other data demonstrate that AMPAR endocytosis is clathrin-mediated [176 ] that synaptic AMPARs are internalized at sites lateral to the PSD [71 ]; and that synaptic AMPARs diffuse laterally to endocytic sites prior to internalization [177 ]. GluA2 is linked to clathrin via the clathrin adaptor protein AP2, which promotes assembly of the clathrin coat [71 , 178 ]. AP2 binds the GluA2 region that overlaps with the binding site for NSF. In fact, NSF-interfering peptides alter GluA2 binding to AP2 [71 ], while NSF has been shown to stabilize AMPARs at the postsynaptic membrane by blocking endocytosis [75 , 179 ]. During NMDA-induced LTD, loss of NSF affinity for GluA2 allowed binding with AP2, in turn resulting in clathrin- dependent endocytosis. Peptides that specifically block the interaction between AP2 and GluA2 impair LTD without affecting the basal synaptic transmission [71 ]. Several proteins interacting with GluA2 are involved in the regulation of AMPAR trafficking in LTD, reviewed in [180 ]. The roles of GRIP1/2 [47 ] and PICK1 [181 , 182 ] are best understood. GRIP1/2 stabilizes GluA2-containing AMPARs at the postsynaptic membranes surface and promotes their recycling after internalization. PICK1 regulates the surface expression of GluA2 by promoting AMPAR internalization in hippocampal neurons in response to Ca2+ influx via NMDARs during LTD [59 , 183 ]. PICK1 also inhibits GluA2 recycling by promoting the retention of GluA2 in the intracellular compartment after internalization [64 , 118 , 172 , 184 ]. Disruption of the GluA2–PICK1 interaction has been shown to block LTD [59 , 172 , 173 , 185 , 186 ]. The affinity of GluA2 for GRIP1/2 or PICK1 is regulated by the phosphorylation status of Ser880 [59 , 61 , 187 ]: Ser880 phosphorylation by PKC is important for AMPAR removal during LTD in hippocampus and cerebellum [187 ], as it induces GluA2 detachment from GRIP1/2 and increases GluA2 affinity for PICK1 [60 , 61 , 172 ]. PICK1 also regulates AMPAR internalization via its inhibition of Arp2/3-mediated actin polymerization [188 ]. Inhibition of actin polymerization favors spine shrinkage and AMPAR endocytosis, with important consequences for both LTD and LTP [174 , 189 , 190 , 191 ]. Tyr876 dephosphorylation is also important for GluA2 internalization during LTD. Tyr876 dephosphorylation, in concert with BRAG2 protein binding to the GluA2 C-terminus, induces BRAG2-mediated Arf6 activation, a process critical for targeted receptor endocytosis [192 ]. A recent study has documented a direct interaction between KIBRA and PICK1 [65 ]. The KIBRA gene is expressed in memory-related brain areas and is involved in human memory performance [193 ]. A single-nucleotide polymorphism (T instead of C) in the ninth intron has been associated with enhanced human performance in episodic memory tasks [193 ]; the T allele is also protective against Alzheimer’s disease [194 ]. Although KIBRA function in the brain has not been fully elucidated, its binding to PICK1, and evidence of its interaction with GluA1/2 subunits, GRIP1 and NSF [65 ], suggest KIBRA involvement in AMPAR trafficking. In addition, KIBRA knockdown in rat hippocampal neurons was shown to accelerate the rate of AMPAR activity-dependent recycling, which was similar to that observed in PICK1 KO neurons [64 ]; while KIBRA KO mice showed strongly impaired LTD and LTP, indicating that KIBRA has a key role in AMPAR trafficking during LTP and LTD [65 ]. The synaptic scaffolding molecule S-SCAM has also been recently shown to be involved in LTD, and in particular to be essential for maintaining GluA2 at the synapse [195 ]. S-SCAM overexpression in hippocampal neurons increases AMPAR expression at the postsynaptic membrane, whereas S-SCAM knockdown reduces AMPAR expression there [195 ]. S-SCAM also plays a key role in activity-dependent AMPAR trafficking, since NMDA-induced AMPAR internalization was blocked in neurons overexpressing S-SCAM; while S-SCAM knockdown reduced the number of excitatory synapses and weakened synaptic transmission [195 ].

AMPAR trafficking in homeostatic synaptic plasticity

The positive feedback mechanisms that characterize Hebbian plasticity, and give rise to LTP and LTD, have the potential to trigger excessive excitation or to completely silence activity. There must therefore be a mechanism capable of maintaining activity within specific physiological ranges, so as to maintain the stability of the neuronal network and the integrity of brain function [196 ]. The identification of homeostatic plasticity at synapses provided a previously unknown control mechanism to prevent destabilization possibly arising from positive feedback [197 , 198 ]. Homeostatic synaptic plasticity is a negative feedback mechanism that counters long-term changes in neuronal activity, restoring them to a physiological set point [199 ]. The compensation is bidirectional: a sustained increase in activity will be compensated for by activity decrease, whereas a chronic decrease will be adjusted in the opposing direction [199 , 200 ]. Homeostatic synaptic plasticity is mediated principally by AMPAR trafficking and in particular by the internalization of AMPARs when the synapse is over-active, and AMPAR exocytosis when the synapse is under-active [200 , 201 ]. Knowledge of homeostatic synaptic plasticity is expanding rapidly, and the emerging picture is complex. Most of the data reviewed below have been obtained from cultured neocortical neurons—a model able to reveal molecular mechanisms but not those pertaining to in vivo connectivity. Nonetheless, studies in which in visual cortex neuronal activity was modulated in vivo [201 , 202 , 203 ] indicate that homeostatic synaptic plasticity is an important physiological phenomenon. Exactly when homeostatic synaptic plasticity takes place remains controversial. While most studies had been performed on young animals, homeostatic synaptic plasticity has also been observed in adults, and although brain area-specific differences have been identified, they need to be better elucidated [203 ].

  • Global versus synapse-specific regulation of AMPAR trafficking in homeostatic plasticity

Homeostatic synaptic plasticity was first described as a mechanism acting on all the synapses of a given neuron, proportionally scaling their strength either up or down (hence the term “synaptic scaling”) [197 , 204 , 205 ]. The classic methods of silencing or hyperactivating an entire network of cultured neurons consist of bath application of, respectively, the sodium channel blocker tetradotoxin (TTX), or the GABA (A) receptor antagonist bicuculline. These treatments result in global homeostatic alterations in synaptic strength, consisting of AMPAR current up-scaling or down-scaling, respectively [199 , 200 , 201 ]. More recently, local mechanisms of homeostatic plasticity have been described [206 , 207 , 208 ]. To assess the synapse specificity of homeostatic plasticity, approaches that selectively inhibit or enhance one or a few synapses are used. Selective synaptic inhibition can be achieved by transfecting a few neurons in culture with the potassium channel Kir2.1 to hyperpolarize the neuron and reduce its firing rate [206 , 207 ]; while selective synapse enhancement can be achieved by transfecting neurons with the light-gated glutamate receptor LiGluR to enhance presynaptic terminal activity on demand by light stimulation [208 ]. The engineered pre-synapse can be visualized by concomitant expression of YFP-synapsin, which makes it possible to identify post-synapses of interest. Using these techniques, it has been shown that single synapses sense their own activity levels and compensate independently of neighboring synapses. Thus, decreases in presynaptic activity are compensated for by recruitment of AMPARs and GRIP1 to the postsynaptic structure, via a mechanism dependent on phosphatidylinositol 3 kinase (PI3K) and the presence of CP-AMPARs (lacking GluA2) [206 ]. Recruitment of the latter receptors is early immediate gene (Arc/Arg3.1)-dependent since recruitment (and compensation) is not observed in Arc/Arg3.1 KO mice. It had previously been shown that Arc/Arg3.1 is important for Hebbian plasticity and global homeostatic scaling [207 ]. Similarly, enhanced presynaptic input has been shown to be counterbalanced by reduced AMPAR abundance at the postsynaptic membrane: initially AMPAR is internalized and subsequently undergoes polyubiquitination through Nedd4 and local proteosomal degradation [208 ]. This mechanism requires NMDAR and calcium signaling but, interestingly, does not rely on classic trafficking pathways such as those mediated by GluN2B NMDAR signaling, calcineurin or CamKII, that are known to be involved in Hebbian plasticity [208]. Presumably, these global and local control systems cooperate to ensure neuronal stability.

  • GluA1 and GluA2 regulation at synapses in homeostatic plasticity

The importance of CP-AMPARs in homeostatic plasticity has been demonstrated only recently [209 , 210 , 211 , 212 ]. Several groups have reported an increase in CP-AMPARs at the postsynaptic membrane following activity deprivation [207 , 213 , 214 , 215 , 216 , 217 ]. Activity deprivation promotes local translation of GluA1 mRNA in the dendrites [215 ], which is dependent on retinoic acid (RA) signaling. In fact, retinoic acid receptor (RAR) alpha binds 5′ UTR mRNA, blocking its translation, while RA is able to remove this block [217 , 218 , 219 ]. The resulting increase in GluA1 synthesis favors the assembly of CP-AMPARs that are then transported to the membrane by a mechanism dependent on CamKIIβ and GluA1 phosphorylation at S845 [202 , 213 , 220 ]. Accumulation of CP-AMPARs at the synapse increases synaptic strength by effects at the presynapse as well as the postsynapse. Postsynaptic calcium entry promotes neurotrophin brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) release, which is responsible for retrograde signaling that enhances the presynaptic release of neurotransmitter by increasing vesicle pool size and turnover rate [220 , 221 ]. These effects lead to a coordinated enhancement of synaptic strength [220 , 221 ]. Various groups have reported that activity deprivation does not increase CP-AMPARs, but increases both GluA1 and GluA2, and that homeostatic plasticity depends on GluA2. Thus Gainey et al. [201 ] reported that TTX treatment caused an increase in AMPARs at synapses in visual cortical neurons both in culture and in vivo, and that this effect was blocked by the GluA2 C-terminal tail or GluA2 shRNA; while interference of GluA1 had no effect, suggesting that GluA2 was specifically required for AMPAR delivery to the postsynaptic membrane in homeostatic plasticity [201 ]. Furthermore, following activity deprivation, the GluA2/3-interacting protein PICK1 is downregulated and GluA2-containing AMPARs are released from the intracellular pool and can reach the surface [222 ]. When PICK1 was knocked down, it abolished the synaptic increase in AMPAR associated with activity deprivation, highlighting the importance of GluA2 regulation in homeostatic plasticity [222 ]. The findings reviewed above therefore suggest the existence of both GluA1- and GluA2-dependent mechanisms in homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Both mechanisms have also been reported in in vivo animal experiments: with synaptic CP-AMPAR accumulation reported following visual deprivation [202 ], and increases in GluA2-containing receptors reported at the synapse following intraocular TTX injection [201 ]. Future studies may be expected to clarify the roles of CP AMPARs and GluA2-containing AMPARs in homeostatic plasticity.

  • Molecules regulating synaptic accumulation of AMPARs in homeostatic plasticity

Several molecules are known to be involved in regulating AMPAR trafficking and synaptic accumulation in homeostatic plasticity. Recently described molecules include soluble factors [BDNF, tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNFα ) and RA]; cell-adhesion molecules (integrin β 3, N-cadherin); PSD scaffolding proteins (PICK1, MAGUKs); intracellular signaling molecules (CamKs, PLK2, PI3K-Akt); proteins related to activity-induced gene expression (Arc/Arg3.1); a protein involved in SUMOylation (SENP1) and the ubiquitin proteasome system (UPS). The following sections summarize the rapidly emerging roles of these molecules.

  • Soluble released factors

BDNF was the first soluble molecule shown to have a role in homeostatic plasticity [197, 223]. In particular, Rutherford et al. [197] showed that low levels of BDNF trigger synaptic up-scaling in cultured cortical pyramidal neurons. In fact, scavenging endogenous BDNF with a soluble form of its receptor (TrkB-IgG) mimicked activity deprivation and induced up-scaling of excitatory synapses; while exogenous BDNF blocked the synaptic up-scaling that follows chronic activity blockade with TTX. BDNF is also important for the pre-synaptic enhancement that follows chronic activity deprivation. Treatment of cultured hippocampal neurons with the AMPAR blocker NBQX causes GluA1 accumulation at the synapse, which creates the conditions for retrograde signaling once the block is removed. Factors essential for this signaling include calcium (which enters through GluA1 homotetramers), BDNF, and NO [221]. AMPAR blockade triggers BDNF synthesis, which drives presynaptic scaling via its presynaptic receptor TrkB. TrkB signaling, together with calcium influx, which enters through P/N/Q presynaptic channels, accelerate synaptic vesicle turnover [224]. TNF-α is a well-known regulator of AMPAR trafficking [225, 226, 227, 228]. Moreover, TNF-α released from glial cells has been shown to be essential for the up-scaling reaction to TTX—a process that does not occur in TNF-α KO mice [229]. The mechanism by which TNF-α exerts these effects is unclear. β3-integrin could be involved, since acute treatment with TNF-α increases surface levels of β3-integrin: this molecule is known to be required for synaptic scaling [230], and its surface expression correlates with the quantity of AMPAR at the post-synaptic membrane [230]. Steinmetz and Turrigiano [231] recently proposed that TNF-α’s role in synaptic scaling is not instructive but permissive: it would maintain synapses in a plastic state that allows synaptic compensation to occur. In support of their hypothesis, it has been found that chronic TNF-α signaling blockade alters PSD composition, increasing SAP102 and decreasing PSD-95 expression [231] so that they resemble immature synapses [101], which would in turn have consequences for AMPAR distribution in the synapse. RA has been identified as a regulator of dendritic protein synthesis in homeostatic plasticity. Following activity blockade with TTX or (2R)-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (APV), RA synthesis is enhanced both in cultured hippocampal neurons and brain slices [217]. In turn, RA enhances the local synthesis of GluA1 (but not GluA2), thus favoring its insertion into the membrane. RA acts through its receptor RARalpha [217] and the fragile X-mental retardation protein FMRP acts downstream of the RA pathway and is necessary for GluA1 synthesis, suggesting that deregulation of homeostatic plasticity might contribute to the pathogenesis of fragile X syndrome [232].

  • Cell adhesion molecules (CAMS)

CAMs are involved in synapse formation and maturation, as well as Hebbian plasticity [233, 234]; their roles in homeostatic synaptic plasticity are emerging rapidly. Integrins are transmembrane adhesion molecules that function as receptors for proteins of the extracellular matrix [235]. Integrin β3 directly binds the C-terminal tail of GluA2 [236], providing a direct link with AMPAR, and has been shown to be specifically required for AMPAR up-scaling in cultured hippocampal neurons following TTX-induced activity deprivation and TNF-α release [230]. The adhesion molecule N-cadherin also interacts with GluA2 [34]. Through its intracellular binding partner β-catenin, N-cadherin regulates AMPAR trafficking [237]. Interestingly, absence of β-catenin from cultured hippocampal neurons, prevents the bidirectional scaling triggered by chronic activity modifications (induced by TTX or bicuculline) [238, 239].


  • PSD scaffolding proteins

Scaffolding proteins are crucial for the structural organization of the PSD. They are also involved in the trafficking of receptors to the postsynaptic membrane and their stabilization there [240]. PICK1 and MAGUK scaffolding proteins have recently been shown to be crucially involved in homeostatic plasticity [222, 241]. Anggono et al. [222] showed that PICK1 is specifically required for inactivity-induced (TTX treatment) AMPAR up-scaling but not for hyperactivity-induced (bicucullin treatment) down-scaling. PICK1 is down-regulated during synaptic up-scaling, enabling GluA2/3 recruitment from the intracellular pool to the membrane. Furthermore, in PICK1 KO neurons, AMPAR composition and trafficking are impaired and the regulatory mechanisms responsible for homeostatic plasticity are compromised. Sun et al. [241] recently explored the role of the MAGUK scaffolding proteins PSD93 and PSD95 in homeostatic plasticity in neocortical pyramidal neurons. These proteins were found to be essential for assembly of the protein–protein association network, which is required for homeostatic adjustments of AMPAR abundance in the synapse. PSD-95 was found necessary for down-scaling, whereas PSD-95 and PSD-93 were both involved in synaptic up-scaling [241].

  • Intracellular signaling molecules

Calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinases (CamKs) are involved in plasticity including homeostatic plasticity [204, 205, 213, 220] through their roles in intracellular calcium signaling [242]. In cultured hippocampal neurons, the α and β isoforms of CamKII are regulated differently by activity: sustained activity shifts the balance in favor of the α isoform, whereas activity deprivation increases the β and decreases the α isoform [213]. In chronic activity deprivation, CamKIIβ seems to drive GluA1 up-scaling [220]. CamKIV is involved bidirectionally in homeostatic plasticity, having been shown to be necessary both for AMPAR up-scaling following activity deprivation [205] and for down-scaling following hyperactivation [204]. The signaling pathway governed by PI3K and Akt has also been shown to be involved in both Hebbian plasticity [243] and homeostatic plasticity [206]. PI3K–Akt regulates AMPAR trafficking [244] and is involved in hippocampal synaptogenesis [245]. PI3K signaling is required for AMPAR accumulation at chronically inhibited single synapses [206]. In presenilin-1-KO neurons—a model of Alzheimer’s disease—the PI3K–Akt cascade is impaired and homeostatic synaptic up-scaling is prevented, suggesting that PI3K–Akt-dependent homeostatic plasticity might be involved in the etiology of Alzheimer’s disease [246]. Polo-like kinase 2 (PLK2) is an activity-inducible serine threonine kinase [247] that was recently shown to favor intracellular retention of AMPAR following hyperactivity. PLK2 directly binds NSF and causes its dissociation from GluA2, thereby favoring the binding of GluA2 to PICK1 and GRIP, and subsequent receptor internalization. PLK2 therefore seems to function as a hyperactivity sensor and contributes to homeostasis by redistributing AMPARs among its adapter proteins NSF, PICK1, and GRIP1 [248].


  • Activity-induced gene expression and protein degradation

The activity-induced immediate early gene product Arc/Arg3.1 (Arc) has a well-established role in homeostatic plasticity [207 , 249 , 250 , 251 ]. Activity regulates Arc, which in turn modulates AMPAR endocytosis through its interaction with the AMPAR endocytic machinery, consisting of endophilin-3 and dynamin-2. In particular, sustained activity correlates with high Arc levels and enhanced AMPAR internalization, while activity deprivation is associated with low Arc levels and reduced receptor internalization [249 , 250 , 251 ]. Craig et al. [252 ] recently showed that post-translational SUMOylation has a role in homeostatic plasticity. In particular, chronic synaptic inactivity reduces levels of the enzyme SENP1, responsible for deSUMOylation, thus favoring protein SUMOylation by the enzyme SUMO1. Since Arc1 is one of the substrates of SUMO1, and its SUMOylation is required for synaptic up-scaling, these findings implicate post-translational modifications of Arc in the regulation of synaptic scaling. The degradation of soluble proteins by the UPS also contributes to homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Proteasome function changes according to neuronal activity: action potential blockade suppresses proteasome function, while hyperactivity increases proteasome activity. The inhibition of proteasome activity is enough to induce AMPAR current up-scaling and to increase GluA1 and GluA2 subunits at the synapse [224 ]. Furthermore, down-regulation of AMPARs as a result of sustained activity has been shown to depend on a local increase in the ubiquitin ligase Nedd4, followed by polyubiquitination and subsequent proteosomal degradation of AMPARs [208 ].


Conclusions

AMPAR trafficking to and from the synapse has been one of the most fascinating and intriguing areas of neuroscience over the past 20 years, as it underlies LTP and LTD—processes that underlie at least some aspects of memory and learning. Much progress has been made in identifying the proteins involved in AMPAR insertion into and removal from the synapse; many of the molecular mechanisms contributing to these events are also clear. However, it is still not clear how the receptors diffuse laterally to and from the synapses, nor is it clear how they are directed to and from the complex of proteins present in the PSD. We also have to remember that most of the information contributing to this progress has come from cultured neocortical neurons from rodents, genetically or otherwise manipulated to KO or overexpress proteins of interest. That such studies cannot provide all the answers are illustrated by the wealth of conflicting results that have been obtained, and most graphically by the recent paper of Granger et al. which, at first sight, seems to have overturned a consensus on the role of AMPAR subunits and their C-terminal tails in LTP that took a wealth of data and 10 years to establish. It would seem that the way forward is to develop new in vivo methods to resolve these contradictions and stimulate further progress in this exciting area of neuroscience.




AMPAR Autoradiography

Determining ad hoc experimental parameters for quantitative autoradiography of AMPAR using [3H]AMPA ligand

Defining the parameters for a quantitative autoradiography experiment can be done three ways: (1) determined experimentally (not necessary for us), (2) taken from an existing protocol (which we did for our first experiment), (3) defined ad hoc using binding affinity data (which I've done below). After surveying the literature and consulting several databases, I've concluded that the binding affinity (dissociation constant Kd) for [3H]AMPA in rodents is ~28 nM. This means that a buffer containing a 28 nM concentration of [3H]AMPA will result in 50% of the total expressed AMPAR being bound to the radioligand. In the first experiment we used a 15 nM concentration; under the law of mass action this should result in 35% of expressed AMPAR being bound to ligand. The calculations for converting radioactivity (specific activity in Curies) to molecular concentrations is theoretically concrete, but converting this to film exposure days is only estimable.


The [3H]AMPA from Sigma is manufactured with the given parameters

  • Total Amount
- 25 uCi or 250 uCi
  • Specific Activity
- 60 Ci/mmol
  • Concentration
- 1 mCi/mL
  • Molecular Weight
- 186.2 g/mol

Radioactivity to Molecular Concentration 1 mCi/ml * 1 mmol/60 Ci * 1 Ci/1000 mCi = 1.67e-5 mmol/mL = 16.7 uM


Sigma sends us a 16.7 uM concentration of [3H]AMPA and we can order either 25 uL ($300) or 250 uL ($1100). We want to determine how many mice brains can be analyzed if we order 25 uL or 250 uL [3H]AMPA. When the tissue is cut using the cryostat it is placed on glass slides. The number of slides in the experiment is key. Using the drop technique (the best way to conserve our precious radioligand) each slide requires 1 mL of incubation buffer (e.g. the Tris buffer containing the [3H]AMPA). All the brain regions of interest can be fit onto 3 slides; therefore there are 3 slides per subject.

Now to compare protocols...


Protocol A -- 15 nM [3H]AMPA
  • 15 nM / (15 nM + 28 nM) = 35% bound receptors
  • 16.7 uM * x mL = .015 uM * 1 mL
  • x = 0.898 uL [3H]AMPA / mL Tris
  • 25 uL / 0.898 uL = 28 slides or 10 subjects
  • 250 uL / 0.898 uL = 278 slides or 92 subjects

At this concentration we are exposing film for 30 days.


Protocol B -- 42 nM [3H]AMPA
  • 42 nM / (42 nM + 28 nM) = 60% bound receptors
  • 16.7 uM * x mL = .042 uM * 1 mL
  • x = 2.5 uL [3H]AMPA / mL Tris
  • 25 uL / 2.5 uL = 10 slides or 3 subjects
  • 250 uL / 2.5 uL = 100 slides or 33 subjects

At this concentration we can estimate a film exposure time of ~21 days


Protocol C -- 100 nM [3H]AMPA
  • 100 nM / (100 nM + 28 nM) = 78% bound receptors
  • 16.7 uM * x mL = .1 uM * 1 mL
  • x = 6.0 uL [3H]AMPA / mL Tris
  • 25 uL / 6.0 uL = 4 slides or 1 subjects
  • 250 uL / 6.0 uL = 41 slides or 14 subjects

At this concentration we can estimate a film exposure time of ~16 days


Thus, I conclude that protocol B would be optimal given we order 250 uCi and protocol A will remain optimal given we order 25 uCi of [3H]AMPA.


Binding Assay 2-amino-3-(3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolyl) propionic acid (AMPA) receptors
PROCEDURES
Radioligand Pre-Incubation Incubation Wash
3H-AMPA
15 nM
50 mM Tris-HCl
100 mM KCl
pH 7.4
10 min
15 nM 3H-AMPA
Pre-Incubation Buffer
60 min
Pre-Incubation Buffer
10 min x2 baths
1 dip diH2O



Experimental determination of receptor-ligand binding affinity for [3H]AMPA

10 nM [3H]AMPA bound 32% of receptors in rat. Frozen-thawed tissue in buffer with KCl bound 36% of receptors. Specific binding curve extrapolation estimates the Kd of AMPA to be ~28 nM (Olsen et al 1987).


Experimental Goals

There is some behavioral evidence that PKMzeta (PKMz) is associated with long term memory maintenance. Molecular studies have shown that PKMz interacts with the GluR2 subunit of AMPA receptors (AMPAR), and with the help of NSF proteins, mediate the trafficking of AMPAR to the membrane.

In a modular fashion, PKMz is localized in dendritic spines. Once activated, PKMz remains constitutively active (much like a prion). In this way, AMPAR can be sustained in high densities at relevant synapses.

ZIP is a designer molecule that has been shown to inactivate PKMz. There are several high-profile reports of successfully using ZIP to abolish a learned behavior. These experiments have failed replication on several accounts. If ZIP inactivates PKMz, this should be reflected by a relatively long-lasting decrease in AMPAR density. This is what I am interested in exploring. I want to know how to effectively use ZIP as a tool to understand memory maintenance and network properties of memory engrams.